Results for 'Bradley C. Love Todd M. Gureckis'

981 found
Order:
  1.  60
    Short Term Gains, Long Term Pains: How Cues About State Aid Learning in Dynamic Environments.Bradley C. Love Todd M. Gureckis - 2009 - Cognition 113 (3):293.
  2.  42
    SUSTAIN: A Network Model of Category Learning.Bradley C. Love, Douglas L. Medin & Todd M. Gureckis - 2004 - Psychological Review 111 (2):309-332.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   114 citations  
  3.  24
    Direct Associations or Internal Transformations? Exploring the Mechanisms Underlying Sequential Learning Behavior.Todd M. Gureckis & Bradley C. Love - 2010 - Cognitive Science 34 (1):10-50.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  4.  90
    When more is less: Feedback effects in perceptual category learning.J. Vincent Filoteo W. Todd Maddox, Bradley C. Love, Brian D. Glass - 2008 - Cognition 108 (2):578.
  5.  17
    You can't play 20 questions with nature and win redux.Bradley C. Love & Robert M. Mok - 2023 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46:e402.
    An incomplete science begets imperfect models. Nevertheless, the target article advocates for jettisoning deep-learning models with some competency in object recognition for toy models evaluated against a checklist of laboratory findings; an approach which evokes Alan Newell's 20 questions critique. We believe their approach risks incoherency and neglects the most basic test; can the model perform its intended task.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  6.  42
    Anticipatory emotions in decision tasks: Covert markers of value or attentional processes?Tyler Davis, Bradley C. Love & W. Todd Maddox - 2009 - Cognition 112 (1):195-200.
    No categories
    Direct download (7 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  7.  70
    The influence of depression symptoms on exploratory decision-making.Nathaniel J. Blanco, A. Ross Otto, W. Todd Maddox, Christopher G. Beevers & Bradley C. Love - 2013 - Cognition 129 (3):563-568.
  8. Thinking in groups.Todd M. Gureckis & Robert L. Goldstone - 2006 - Pragmatics and Cognition 14 (2):293-311.
    Is cognition an exclusive property of the individual or can groups have a mind of their own? We explore this question from the perspective of complex adaptive systems. One of the principal insights from this line of work is that rules that govern behavior at one level of analysis can cause qualitatively different behavior at higher levels. We review a number of behavioral studies from our lab that demonstrate how groups of people interacting in real-time can self-organize into adaptive, problem-solving (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   14 citations  
  9. Active learning strategies in a spatial concept learning game.Todd M. Gureckis & Doug Markant - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn, Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. pp. 3145--3150.
  10. The effect of the internal structure of categories on perception.Todd M. Gureckis & Robert L. Goldstone - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky, Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 1876--1881.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   10 citations  
  11. How You Named Your Child: Understanding the Relationship Between Individual Decision Making and Collective Outcomes.Todd M. Gureckis & Robert L. Goldstone - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (4):651-674.
    We examine the interdependence between individual and group behavior surrounding a somewhat arbitrary, real‐world decision: selecting a name for one’s child. Using a historical database of the names given to children over the last century in the United States, we find that naming choices are influenced by both the frequency of a name in the general population, and by its ‘‘momentum’’ in the recent past in the sense that names which are growing in popularity are preferentially chosen. This bias toward (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  12. Collective Behavior.Robert L. Goldstone & Todd M. Gureckis - 2009 - Topics in Cognitive Science 1 (3):412-438.
    The resurgence of interest in collective behavior is in large part due to tools recently made available for conducting laboratory experiments on groups, statistical methods for analyzing large data sets reflecting social interactions, the rapid growth of a diverse variety of online self‐organized collectives, and computational modeling methods for understanding both universal and scenario‐specific social patterns. We consider case studies of collective behavior along four attributes: the primary motivation of individuals within the group, kinds of interactions among individuals, typical dynamics (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   22 citations  
  13.  59
    European and American Philosophers.John Marenbon, Douglas Kellner, Richard D. Parry, Gregory Schufreider, Ralph McInerny, Andrea Nye, R. M. Dancy, Vernon J. Bourke, A. A. Long, James F. Harris, Thomas Oberdan, Paul S. MacDonald, Véronique M. Fóti, F. Rosen, James Dye, Pete A. Y. Gunter, Lisa J. Downing, W. J. Mander, Peter Simons, Maurice Friedman, Robert C. Solomon, Nigel Love, Mary Pickering, Andrew Reck, Simon J. Evnine, Iakovos Vasiliou, John C. Coker, Georges Dicker, James Gouinlock, Paul J. Welty, Gianluigi Oliveri, Jack Zupko, Tom Rockmore, Wayne M. Martin, Ladelle McWhorter, Hans-Johann Glock, Georgia Warnke, John Haldane, Joseph S. Ullian, Steven Rieber, David Ingram, Nick Fotion, George Rainbolt, Thomas Sheehan, Gerald J. Massey, Barbara D. Massey, David E. Cooper, David Gauthier, James M. Humber, J. N. Mohanty, Michael H. Dearmey, Oswald O. Schrag, Ralf Meerbote, George J. Stack, John P. Burgess, Paul Hoyningen-Huene, Nicholas Jolley, Adriaan T. Peperzak, E. J. Lowe, William D. Richardson, Stephen Mulhall & C. - 1991 - In Robert L. Arrington, A Companion to the Philosophers. Malden, Mass.: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 109–557.
    Peter Abelard (1079–1142 ce) was the most wide‐ranging philosopher of the twelfth century. He quickly established himself as a leading teacher of logic in and near Paris shortly after 1100. After his affair with Heloise, and his subsequent castration, Abelard became a monk, but he returned to teaching in the Paris schools until 1140, when his work was condemned by a Church Council at Sens. His logical writings were based around discussion of the “Old Logic”: Porphyry's Isagoge, aristotle'S Categories and (...)
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  14. Category learning through active sampling.Doug Markant & Todd M. Gureckis - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone, Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society. pp. 248--253.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  28
    Memory enhancements from active control of learning emerge across development.Azzurra Ruggeri, Douglas B. Markant, Todd M. Gureckis, Maria Bretzke & Fei Xu - 2019 - Cognition 186 (C):82-94.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   8 citations  
  16.  39
    Causal Information‐Seeking Strategies Change Across Childhood and Adolescence.Kate Nussenbaum, Alexandra O. Cohen, Zachary J. Davis, David J. Halpern, Todd M. Gureckis & Catherine A. Hartley - 2020 - Cognitive Science 44 (9):e12888.
    Intervening on causal systems can illuminate their underlying structures. Past work has shown that, relative to adults, young children often make intervention decisions that appear to confirm a single hypothesis rather than those that optimally discriminate alternative hypotheses. Here, we investigated how the ability to make informative causal interventions changes across development. Ninety participants between the ages of 7 and 25 completed 40 different puzzles in which they had to intervene on various causal systems to determine their underlying structures. Each (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  17.  31
    Self‐Directed Learning Favors Local, Rather Than Global, Uncertainty.Douglas B. Markant, Burr Settles & Todd M. Gureckis - 2016 - Cognitive Science 40 (1):100-120.
    Collecting information that one expects to be useful is a powerful way to facilitate learning. However, relatively little is known about how people decide which information is worth sampling over the course of learning. We describe several alternative models of how people might decide to collect a piece of information inspired by “active learning” research in machine learning. We additionally provide a theoretical analysis demonstrating the situations under which these models are empirically distinguishable, and we report a novel empirical study (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  18. Is categorical perception really verbally mediated perception?Andrew T. Hendrickson, George Kachergis, Todd M. Gureckis & Robert L. Goldstone - 2010 - In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone, Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   4 citations  
  19.  84
    The Algorithmic Level Is the Bridge Between Computation and Brain.Bradley C. Love - 2015 - Topics in Cognitive Science 7 (2):230-242.
    Every scientist chooses a preferred level of analysis and this choice shapes the research program, even determining what counts as evidence. This contribution revisits Marr's three levels of analysis and evaluates the prospect of making progress at each individual level. After reviewing limitations of theorizing within a level, two strategies for integration across levels are considered. One is top–down in that it attempts to build a bridge from the computational to algorithmic level. Limitations of this approach include insufficient theoretical constraint (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   15 citations  
  20.  83
    When learning to classify by relations is easier than by features.Bradley C. Love & Marc T. Tomlinson - 2010 - Thinking and Reasoning 16 (4):372-401.
  21. Modeling item and category learning.Bradley C. Love & Douglas L. Medin - 1998 - In Morton Ann Gernsbacher & Sharon J. Derry, Proceedings of the 20th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Lawerence Erlbaum. pp. 639--644.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  22.  17
    Mutability, conceptual transformation, and context.Bradley C. Love - 1996 - In Garrison W. Cottrell, Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of The Cognitive Science Society. Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 459--463.
    No categories
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  23. Predicting information needs: Adaptive display in dynamic environments.Bradley C. Love, Matt Jones, Marc T. Tomlinson & Michael Howe - 2008 - In B. C. Love, K. McRae & V. M. Sloutsky, Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Cognitive Science Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  24. Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition.Matt Jones & Bradley C. Love - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4):169-188.
    The prominence of Bayesian modeling of cognition has increased recently largely because of mathematical advances in specifying and deriving predictions from complex probabilistic models. Much of this research aims to demonstrate that cognitive behavior can be explained from rational principles alone, without recourse to psychological or neurological processes and representations. We note commonalities between this rational approach and other movements in psychology – namely, Behaviorism and evolutionary psychology – that set aside mechanistic explanations or make use of optimality assumptions. Through (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   131 citations  
  25. Concept learning.Bradley C. Love - 2003 - In L. Nadel, Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Nature Publishing Group.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  26.  42
    Grounding quantum probability in psychological mechanism.Bradley C. Love - 2013 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (3):296-296.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  27.  20
    Model comparison, not model falsification.Bradley C. Love - 2018 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 41.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  28.  19
    Mechanistic models of associative and rule-based category learning.Bradley C. Love & Marc Tomlinson - 2010 - In Denis Mareschal, Paul Quinn & Stephen E. G. Lea, The Making of Human Concepts. Oxford University Press. pp. 53--74.
  29.  34
    Three deadly sins of category learning modelers.Bradley C. Love - 2001 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (4):687-688.
    Tenenbaum and Griffiths's article continues three disturbing trends that typify category learning modeling: (1) modelers tend to focus on a single induction task; (2) the drive to create models that are formally elegant has resulted in a gross simplification of the phenomena of interest; (3) related research is generally ignored when doing so is expedient. [Tenenbaum & Griffiths].
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
  30.  51
    Feature Centrality and Conceptual Coherence.Steven A. Sloman, Bradley C. Love & Woo-Kyoung Ahn - 1998 - Cognitive Science 22 (2):189-228.
    Conceptual features differ in how mentally tranformable they are. A robin that does not eat is harder to imagine than a robin that does not chirp. We argue that features are immutable to the extent that they are central in a network of dependency relations. The immutability of a feature reflects how much the internal structure of a concept depends on that feature; i.e., how much the feature contributes to the concept's coherence. Complementarily, mutability reflects the aspects in which a (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   80 citations  
  31.  46
    A theory of the electrical properties of liquid metals II. Polyvalent metals.C. C. Bradley, T. E. Faber, E. G. Wilson & J. M. Ziman - 1962 - Philosophical Magazine 7 (77):865-887.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   20 citations  
  32.  56
    CAB: Connectionist Analogy Builder.Levi B. Larkey & Bradley C. Love - 2003 - Cognitive Science 27 (5):781-794.
    The ability to make informative comparisons is central to human cognition. Comparison involves aligning two representations and placing their elements into correspondence. Detecting correspondences is a necessary component of analogical inference, recognition, categorization, schema formation, and similarity judgment. Connectionist Analogy Builder (CAB) determines correspondences through a simple iterative computation that matches elements in one representation with elements playing compatible roles in the other representation while simultaneously enforcing structural constraints. CAB shows promise as a process model of comparison as its performance (...)
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   19 citations  
  33.  25
    When unsupervised training benefits category learning.Franziska Bröker, Bradley C. Love & Peter Dayan - 2022 - Cognition 221 (C):104984.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  34.  25
    How decisions and the desire for coherency shape subjective preferences over time.Adam N. Hornsby & Bradley C. Love - 2020 - Cognition 200 (C):104244.
    No categories
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  35.  45
    Pinning down the theoretical commitments of Bayesian cognitive models.Matt Jones & Bradley C. Love - 2011 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34 (4):215-231.
    Mathematical developments in probabilistic inference have led to optimism over the prospects for Bayesian models of cognition. Our target article calls for better differentiation of these technical developments from theoretical contributions. It distinguishes between Bayesian Fundamentalism, which is theoretically limited because of its neglect of psychological mechanism, and Bayesian Enlightenment, which integrates rational and mechanistic considerations and is thus better positioned to advance psychological theory. The commentaries almost uniformly agree that mechanistic grounding is critical to the success of the Bayesian (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  36.  21
    Bidirectional influences of information sampling and concept learning.Kurt Braunlich & Bradley C. Love - 2022 - Psychological Review 129 (2):213-234.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  37. You only had to ask me once: Long-term retention requires direct queries during learning.Yasuaki Sakamoto & Bradley C. Love - 2009 - In N. A. Taatgen & H. van Rijn, Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society.
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  38.  36
    Monkey see, monkey do: Learning relations through concrete examples.Marc T. Tomlinson & Bradley C. Love - 2008 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31 (2):150-151.
    Penn et al. argue that the complexity of relational learning is beyond animals. We discuss a model that demonstrates relational learning need not involve complex processes. Novel stimuli are compared to previous experiences stored in memory. As learning shifts attention from featural to relational cues, the comparison process becomes more analogical in nature, successfully accounting for performance across species and development.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  39.  64
    Enhancement of cognitive control by approach and avoidance motivational states.Adam C. Savine, Stefanie M. Beck, Bethany G. Edwards, Kimberly S. Chiew & Todd S. Braver - 2010 - Cognition and Emotion 24 (2):338-356.
    Affective variables have been shown to impact working memory and cognitive control. Theoretical arguments suggest that the functional impact of emotion on cognition might be mediated through shifting action dispositions related to changes in motivational orientation. The current study examined the effects of positive and negative affect on performance via direct manipulation of motivational state in tasks with high demands on cognitive control. Experiment 1 examined the effects of monetary reward on task-switching performance, while Experiment 2 examined the effects of (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   13 citations  
  40.  23
    Strength of Ventral Tegmental Area Connections With Left Caudate Nucleus Is Related to Conflict Monitoring.Ping C. Mamiya, Todd Richards, Neva M. Corrigan & Patricia K. Kuhl - 2020 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  41.  51
    The Nature of Belief-Directed Exploratory Choice in Human Decision-Making.W. Bradley Knox, A. Ross Otto, Peter Stone & Bradley C. Love - 2011 - Frontiers in Psychology 2.
  42.  88
    Structural Priming as Structure-Mapping: Children Use Analogies From Previous Utterances to Guide Sentence Production.Micah B. Goldwater, Marc T. Tomlinson, Catharine H. Echols & Bradley C. Love - 2011 - Cognitive Science 35 (1):156-170.
    What mechanisms underlie children’s language production? Structural priming—the repetition of sentence structure across utterances—is an important measure of the developing production system. We propose its mechanism in children is the same as may underlie analogical reasoning: structure-mapping. Under this view, structural priming is the result of making an analogy between utterances, such that children map semantic and syntactic structure from previous to future utterances. Because the ability to map relationally complex structures develops with age, younger children are less successful than (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   7 citations  
  43.  11
    System alignment supports cross-domain learning and zero-shot generalisation.Kaarina Aho, Brett D. Roads & Bradley C. Love - 2022 - Cognition 227 (C):105200.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  44.  25
    Differential Synchronization in Default and Task-Specific Networks of the Human Brain.Aaron Kirschner, Julia Wing Yan Kam, Todd C. Handy & Lawrence M. Ward - 2012 - Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6.
  45.  25
    Moderate Reverberation Does Not Increase Subjective Fatigue, Subjective Listening Effort, or Behavioral Listening Effort in School-Aged Children.Erin M. Picou, Brianna Bean, Steven C. Marcrum, Todd A. Ricketts & Benjamin W. Y. Hornsby - 2019 - Frontiers in Psychology 10.
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  46.  33
    History of American Political Thought.John Agresto, John E. Alvis, Donald R. Brand, Paul O. Carrese, Laurence D. Cooper, Murray Dry, Jean Bethke Elshtain, Thomas S. Engeman, Christopher Flannery, Steven Forde, David Fott, David F. Forte, Matthew J. Franck, Bryan-Paul Frost, David Foster, Peter B. Josephson, Steven Kautz, John Koritansky, Peter Augustine Lawler, Howard L. Lubert, Harvey C. Mansfield, Jonathan Marks, Sean Mattie, James McClellan, Lucas E. Morel, Peter C. Meyers, Ronald J. Pestritto, Lance Robinson, Michael J. Rosano, Ralph A. Rossum, Richard S. Ruderman, Richard Samuelson, David Lewis Schaefer, Peter Schotten, Peter W. Schramm, Kimberly C. Shankman, James R. Stoner, Natalie Taylor, Aristide Tessitore, William Thomas, Daryl McGowan Tress, David Tucker, Eduardo A. Velásquez, Karl-Friedrich Walling, Bradley C. S. Watson, Melissa S. Williams, Delba Winthrop, Jean M. Yarbrough & Michael Zuckert - 2003 - Lexington Books.
    This book is a collection of secondary essays on America's most important philosophic thinkers—statesmen, judges, writers, educators, and activists—from the colonial period to the present. Each essay is a comprehensive introduction to the thought of a noted American on the fundamental meaning of the American regime.
    Direct download  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  47.  55
    SMART, J. J. C.: "Philosophy and scientific realism".M. C. Bradley - 1964 - Australasian Journal of Philosophy 42:262.
  48.  78
    Attentional biases for emotional faces.B. P. Bradley, K. Mogg, N. Millar, C. Bonham-Carter, E. Fergusson, J. Jenkins & M. Parr - 1997 - Cognition and Emotion 11 (1):25-42.
  49.  61
    Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart.Gerd Gigerenzer, Peter M. Todd & A. B. C. Research Group - 1999 - New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press USA. Edited by Peter M. Todd.
    Simple Heuristics That Make Us Smart invites readers to embark on a new journey into a land of rationality that differs from the familiar territory of cognitive science and economics. Traditional views of rationality tend to see decision makers as possessing superhuman powers of reason, limitless knowledge, and all of eternity in which to ponder choices. To understand decisions in the real world, we need a different, more psychologically plausible notion of rationality, and this book provides it. It is about (...)
    Direct download (2 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   265 citations  
  50.  43
    Retracted article: Systematic assessment of research on autism spectrum disorder and mercury reveals conflicts of interest and the need for transparency in autism research.Janet K. Kern, David A. Geier, Richard C. Deth, Lisa K. Sykes, Brian S. Hooker, James M. Love, Geir Bjørklund, Carmen G. Chaigneau, Boyd E. Haley & Mark R. Geier - 2017 - Science and Engineering Ethics 23 (6):1689-1690.
    Historically, entities with a vested interest in a product that critics have suggested is harmful have consistently used research to back their claims that the product is safe. Prominent examples are: tobacco, lead, bisphenol A, and atrazine. Research literature indicates that about 80–90 % of studies with industry affiliation found no harm from the product, while only about 10–20 % of studies without industry affiliation found no harm. In parallel to other historical debates, recent studies examining a possible relationship between (...)
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   1 citation  
1 — 50 / 981